perm filename OBJMEA[4,KMC] blob
sn#155782 filedate 1975-04-18 generic text, type T, neo UTF8
THE OBJECTIVE MEASUREMENT OF NONSPEAKING CHILDRENS' INTERACTIONS
WITH A COMPUTER-CONTROLLED PROGRAM FOR THE
STIMULATION OF LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT
KENNETH MARK COLBY
AND
HELENA C. KRAEMER
One difficulty in evaluating treatment improvements in
disorders whose defining characteristics are purely behavioral is
that we lack objective measurements. An objective measurement is one
which is intersubjectively confirmable and impartially independent of
individual opinions, intuitions, and judgemments. When humans try to
be both participants in and observer-recorders of, their
interactions, objective measurement becomes difficult. But when
one participant in an interaction is a machine such as a computer, an
opportunity arises for the machine itself to record observations and
to collect data. This capacity of a computer allows us to develop a
measurement standard , a basis of comparison in which interactions
can be considered similar or different according to objectively
defined measurement criteria.
While developing a computer-aided treatment method for
stimulating language behavior in nonspeaking autistic children (Colby
and Smith,1971,Colby,1973), we became interested in how the
interactions between these children and the machine differed from
those of (1) normal children and (2) other types of nonspeaking
children. To evaluate the treatment method we attempted to follow
changes in interactions over time and to assess whether these changes
could be considered as an improvement. If no changes occurred or if
the changes were judged as retrogressive, then the treatment should
be discontinued. We chose to define improvement as a change in
interactions towards those characteristic of normal speaking
children.
The treatment involves letting a child play with a
computer-controlled audio-visual device consisting of a
typewriter-like keyboard and a television-like video display screen.
When a child presses a key, a symbol, letter, word, expression or
drawing appears on the screen accompanied by a sound, mainly human
voice sounds and some animal or machine sounds. The overall program
is divided into "games" of varying complexity. A sitter who sits with
the child changes the games in accordance with the child's interests
and abilities. The simplest game is Game #1 in which pressing a key
produces that key's symbol on the video screen accompanied by a voice
pronouncing the corresponding letter or number. We shall limit our
discussion of objective measurement to the data collected in this one
game.
To keep track of the child's interactions with the machine, a
program was written (by Earl Sacerdoti, a graduate student in the
Department of Computer Science, Stanford University) which recorded
the game the child was playing, which key was pressed at what exact
time and whether the sound for this key was played over the audio
device. The computer-controlled system is designed so that if a
child presses a single key or several keys in bursts of less than a
second's duration, the sound for the first pressing is played but the
rest are not, in order to avoid confusing the child. As soon as the
child pauses at the end of such a burst, a buffer is cleared of all
the symbols accumulated during the burst, and when the next key is
pressed, its sound is played. Striking the keys at an extremely rapid
rate indicates a child is ignoring the sound and paying attention
only to his visual and/or tactile experience.
In Fig. 1 the first column on the left indicates the number
of the game, the second column indicates the key struck, the third
column shows the time of striking in hours-minutes-seconds, and the
fourth shows whether the sound for that key was played ("played") or
not (blank space). The data of Fig. 1 illustrates the interaction of
a normal speaking 17 month old boy. Fig.2 shows the interactions of
an eight year old nonspeaking boy clinically diagnosed as autistic
and with a score of +26 on Rimland's E2 scale. (A score greater
than +20 is considered by Rimland to indicate a true case of early
infantile autism (Rimland,1971)). It is noteworthy how, on simple
visual inspection, the data of the 8 year old autistic child
resembles that of a 17 month old normal child. In this way an
objective measurement allows us to establish equivalences between
different children. Fig.3 demonstrates the interactions of a normal
speaking 4 year old boy which are strikingly different from the other
two children.
To analyze data from several children playing Game #1, we
examined for each child (1) the total number of runs (a run
consisting of the same key being pressed),(2) the distribution of the
lengths of runs, (4) the number of gaps (blank spaces indicating
non-listening to sound),and (5) the distribution of the length of the
gaps. From these observed values we computed statistics descriptive
of the performance of the child and relatively independent of the
length of the game. These were (1) average run and gap length, (2)
relative frequency distributions of runs and gaps, and (3) numbers of
gaps per length of test. Discarded from this list were those
measures which for normal children were insensitve to the age of the
child. The two most sensitive measures were found to be: (1)
proportion of runs of length 1 (%R1) and (2) relative number of gaps
(total number of gaps divided by total length of runs). The data for
these measures from fifteen normal and three types of nonspeaking
children (autistic, aphasic, organic brain syndrome) appears in
Tables 1. and 2.
From the normal children data we can construct a prediction
line as shown in the graph of Fig. 4. Normal children from age 17
months to 10 years appear to progress in the performance
characteristics of their interactions along this line. One can
project the observed point describing a normal child's performance
onto the prediction line, and, as can be seen, the projection points
with but one exception, line up according to the age of the child. If
one graphs the abscissa of the projection against the age of the
child, one can produce an age-prediction curve based on the
performance characteristics. (See Fig. 5).
Using curve fitting procedures on these data we found that
the numerical formula for prediction was:
A(i) = 0.960 - 1.91 log (1 - % R1 + 0.809 TNG/TLR)
For each normal and each nonspeaking child, the age-level of
performance was computed by this formula and appears in Tables 1 and
2. In the age range of particular interest (1 -4 years), the
age-level of the performance of normal children is closely comparable
to their actual chronological age. On the other hand, the
performances of the nonspeaking children are comparable to normal
children less than 4 years of age. Over time it can be assessed
whether a nonspeaking child is progressing towards more normal
interactions (i.e. achieving higher age-level performances), whether
he has reached a plateau, or is retrogressing.
As yet we do not have complete data from start to finish of
treatment on an improved case of a nonspeaking autistic child.
However, Fig. 6 shows some current (Aug.l973) interactions of D., a
nonspeaking autistic 10 year old boy whom we treated for two years
three years ago and whose language development gained markedly. (We
were not collecting this type of data when he was in treatment.)
Stretches of D's interactions are quite normal looking and his
location on the age-equivalent curve of Fig. 5 shows him to be
performing slightly better than a normal three year old. (It should
be noted that between ages 3-4 a normal child achieves grammatical
complexity roughly equivalent to adult colloquial language, making
occasional mistakes. Between ages 4-5 language becomes fully
established with all adult grammatical forms, deviating only in
style).
We have several cases of failure in which the final sessions
of treatment show interactions indistinguishable from those of the
early sessions. The data of the child in Fig.2 over the past two
years reveal short periods of retrogression and no overall
improvement. We will try for another year and if no change occurs,
we will discontinue. We must also be prepared for the possibilities
that (1) a child's comprehension and speech improves as judged by
clinical and parental observation but his interactions do not change
or (2) a child's interactions change towards the normal but he still
does not use speech for social communication. As yet, we have not
observed either of these paradoxical outcomes.
In summary, we have presented an objective measurement of
children's interactions in playing with a computer-controlled
audio-visual device programmed to stimulate language development.
This measurement is useful in 3 ways: (1) it reveals where a child
stands on an interaction curve relative to normal and other non-
speaking children; thus treatment can be planned to suit the child's
position on an age-equivalent curve, (2) changes in the interactions
over time can be evaluated to see if a child is improving or not, and
(3) if no change takes place or a child reaches a plateau and remains
there for a long time, discontinuation of the treatment can be
justified. Thus an objective measurement of interactions serves as
a useful instrument in planning and terminating treatment of language
deficiencies in nonspeaking children.
References
Colby, K. M. (1973). The rationale for computer-based treatment of
language difficulties in nonspeaking autistic children.
Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 3, 254-260.
Colby, K. M. and Smith, D. C. (1971). Computers in the treatment
of nonspeaking autistic children. In J. H. Masserman
(Ed.), Current Psychiatric Therapies, Grune & Stratton,
N.Y.
Rimland, B. (1971). The differentiation of childhood psychoses: an
analysis of checklists for 2,218 children. Journal of
Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, 175-189.